HOME JOURNALS CONTACT

Pakistan Journal of Biological Sciences

Year: 2010 | Volume: 13 | Issue: 9 | Page No.: 431-436
DOI: 10.3923/pjbs.2010.431.436
Effect of Bright Light on Shift Work Nurses in Hospitals
Z. Zamanian, H. Kakooei, S.M.T. Ayattollahi and M. Dehghani

Abstract: The aim of this study are to assess, in a hospital setting, the effects of Bright Light (BL) on the rhythms in body temperature, plasma melatonin, plasma cortisol and subjective alertness during shift work. In our experimental design, 34 healthy shift work nurses from a university hospital were exposed to bright light (4500 lux) during two break times (21:15 to 22; 00 and 3:15 to 4:00) for four consecutive weeks. In this survey, the subjects were studied under 24 h of realistic conditions during which their plasma cortisol and plasma melatonin was measured at 3 h intervals. In addition, their body temperatures were measured during and after night shift work. Subjective alertness and fatigue were evaluated with the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) and Visual Analog Scale (VOI). It was found that bright light administration significantly suppressed nighttime melatonin levels during night shift, most strongly at 2:00 a.m. A one-way ANOVA, with repeated measurement design, revealed that Bright Light (BL) tended to increase cortisol levels and body temperature and improved alertness significantly during night shift. These results demonstrate that photic stimulation in a hospital setting can have a powerful influence on the adjustment of the circadian system.

Fulltext PDF Fulltext HTML

How to cite this article
Z. Zamanian, H. Kakooei, S.M.T. Ayattollahi and M. Dehghani, 2010. Effect of Bright Light on Shift Work Nurses in Hospitals. Pakistan Journal of Biological Sciences, 13: 431-436.

Keywords: bright light, circadian rhythms, shift work, hospital and Nurse

INTRODUCTION

Humans have evolved for many millions of years on Earth with a reliable daily cycle of 12 h of bright full-spectrum light and 12 h of dark (season and latitude permitting). In fact, this cycle has been a major formative factor for all life on the planet for 3 billion years (or perhaps 4 billion years) (Nemchin et al., 2008). When Thomas Edison invented the light bulb around 1880, who would have thought that it would change the very nature of the workplace (Jaffe et al., 1996). Over the past 100 years, we have cut our nightly sleep by more than 20% and in the last 20 years we have added about 158 h (nearly a full month) to our commuting and annual work schedules (Scott, 1994). Inherent in this discussion is talk about body clocks, a term that refers to the patterns of energy and exhaustion, functioning and resting and wakefulness and sleep that characterize everyday life. This concept of the body clock is in reference to circadian rhythms. A biological clock is any sort of mechanism internal to an organism that governs its biological rhythms. One such mechanism is the pineal gland. The pineal gland secretes two hormones (molecules that send signals to the body), melatonin and serotonin. It appears that melatonin regulates a number of diurnal, or daily, events (Khalsa et al., 2003). Melatonin suppression have received scrutiny including alteration of clock gene functioning and desynchronization of the master clock in the suprachiasmatic nuclei from the peripheral clocks in tissue (Vujovic et al., 2008; Stevens et al., 2007).

Until recently, chronobiologists agreed that social cues were the main zeitgebers for human circadian rhythms and that the light-dark cycle, which is the most important zeitgeber for animals, was relatively unimportant for humans (Wever et al., 1983). This view changed radically, however, after the discovery that exposure to bright light suppresses nighttime melatonin production; whereas ordinary room light is not sufficiently intense to be effective (Lewy et al., 1980). Emergency services that involve continuous operations are particularly vulnerable to shift related fatigue. Compared with only 4% of day workers, 83% of nightshift workers report feeling tired and worker capacity typically declines between 5-10% (Bonnet, 1990).

Increasing numbers of people must do shift work in modern societies and few people will give up electri lighting at home. An understanding of what particular characteristics of wavelength, intensity, timing and duration most disrupt circadian rhythms would permit a minimization of any potential health risks (Hunt and Sassone-Corsi, 2007).

There are nearly half million workers in the Iran who regularly work at shift work. Many are employed in occupations in which peak functioning is critical (e.g., nurses and physicians, airline pilots, industrial workers, polices and heavy machinery and commercial drivers) (Zamanian et al., 2008).

In most health care systems, nurses are the largest working group and play a major role in the system’s success. As health care providers, nurses are obliged to work during day and night to cater for needs of sick people. This can only be possible if nursing services are provided around the clock (Hawley, 1992). Studies have shown that shift-work has negative impact on job performance, sleep, physical and emotional health, social and family life, drug use and level of job-related stress (Brown-DeGagne and Eskes, 1998). Estryn-Behar et al. (1990), in a study conducted on stress at work and mental health status among female hospital workers, concluded that the evidence of association between work involving an existing accumulation of stress factors and mental well-being necessitated consideration in interventions aimed at improving working conditions of hospital workers.

Shift work, particularly that involving night work, therefore, presents a challenge to the internal de-synchronization of circadian rhythms. This study assessed, in a hospital setting, the effects of Bright Light (BL) on the rhythms in body temperature, plasma melatonin, plasma cortisol and subjective alertness among nurses during shift work.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Shift work, particularly that involving night work, presents a challenge to the internal de-synchronization of circadian rhythms. This study assessed, in a hospital setting, the effects of Bright Light (BL) on the rhythms in body temperature, plasma melatonin, plasma cortisol and subjective alertness during shift work in October 2007 to August 2009.

Aims

Determination of the amount of illumination in nursing stations
Determination of nurses’ mental and physical health in different wards
Determination of the effect of Bright light on the intrusive hormone secretion in circadian rhythm
Determination of the relationship between Bright light and nurses’ attention
Determination of the effect of shift work on the secretion of circadian rhythm hormones

We had several steps in this study. In the first step, of 1396 registered nurses employed in all 12 hospitals in Shiraz, 1195 nurses, including 1064 female and 131 male, partook in this study. A 28 item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-28) was used as a self-administered screening tool for detection of mental disorders. This questionnaire was developed by Goldberg and Hillier (1979) for screening somatic symptoms, anxiety and insomnia, social dysfunction and severe depression (Saiepour et al., 2008). In these hospitals we measured illuminance in nurse rest rooms and their stations.

Secondly, we observed shift work patterns and it became apparent that

Job tenure (years) under 5 (40 h work every one week): 8 night work every month
Job tenure (years) 5-10 (36 h work every one week): 6 night work every month
Job tenure (years) 10-15 (36 h work every one week): 4 night work every month
Job tenure (years) 15-20 (36 h work every one week): 2 night work every mont
Job tenure (years) upper 20 (36 h work every one week): 0 night work every month
11 h night shift work, 7.5 h morning and afternoon shifts

Demographic characteristics of the first step of study population are presented in Table 1. The mean (SD) age of nurses was 30.81 (7.08) years; 89.04% of the participants was female and 10.96% was male. One thousand and seventy three of the nurses studied (89.79%) worked on rotating shift system and 122 nurses (10.21%) worked on fixed shift (day time). The mean (SD) job tenure of the nurses in the current hospital was found to be 7.28 (SD = 6.83) years. 50.46% of the participants were married and 62.85% worked in non-governmental hospitals (Table 1).

In the next step in our experimental design, 34 healthy shift work nurses from a university hospital were exposed to very BL (4500 lux) or normal light (300 lux) during two break times (21:15 to 22; 00 and 3:15 to 4:00) for 4 consecutive weeks.

Table 1: Some personal details of the nurses who participated in the study (n = 1195)

The subjects were studied under 24 h of realistic conditions during which their plasma cortisol (RIA method) and plasma melatonin (RIA method). This assay has a sensitivity of 2.5 pg mL-1, an intera-assay coefficient of variation averaged 13.8 % for values <10 pg mL-1, 8.6% in the range of 10-30 pg mL-1 and 5.2% for values >30 mg mL-1. Values reported represent the means of two determinations for each sample. It was measured at 3 h intervals. In addition, their body temperatures were measured during and after night shift work. Subjective alertness was evaluated with the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) and fatigue was evaluated with Visual Analog Scale (VAS). This was selected because it is known to vary with the circadian rhythm of body temperature and to be sensitive to sleep loss (Dijk et al., 1992; Dinges et al., 1997; Cajochen et al., 1999; Wyatt et al., 1999).

The results were gathered in two situations: normal light (300 lux) bright light (4500 lux). Bright light administration was found to have significantly suppressed nighttime melatonin levels during night shift, most strongly at 2:00 am.

Statistical methods: A one-way ANOVA with repeated measurement design revealed that BL tended to increase cortisol levels and body temperature and improved the alertness and fatigue significantly during night shift. These results demonstrate that photic stimulation in hospital setting can have a powerful influence on the adjustment to the circadian system.

RESULTS

Based on these findings, the mean lighting intensity in nursing stations is usually 315 lux, which is suitable for semi-accurate works.

Table 2: Mean luminosity in nurse rest rooms, stations and corridors in shift work times

Fig. 1: Variations in nurse body temperature

Fig. 2: Nurses plasma melatonin variations

But in these locations nurses often complete patient’s records that can be considered as an accurate work, so because of the low light intensity, vision problems, fatigue, lack of concentration and reduction of consciousness are not unexpected to occur in nurses (Table 2).

This study shows that the temperature trend in bright light exposures and in normal lighting conditions, the maximum temperature is in 5-8 pm and when the nurses were exposed to bright light their body temperature increased 0.1°C (Fig. 1).

Figure 2 compares the model of changes of the mean melatonin concentration in the two conditions (in bright light exposures and in normal lighting conditions). It is clear that the overall model of the changes is similar. The concentration of melatonin decreases immediately after bright light contact and this decrease continues until 5 am. After 5 am, the melatonin concentration in plasma increases and from 7 to 10 am, the melatonin concentration in nurses when they are exposed to bright light is higher than when they were in normal lighting conditions (Lewy et al., 1998).

By comparing Fig. 1 and 2, we can deduce that the maximum amount of melatonin secretion is related to the time in which the body temperature is the lowest.

Fig. 3: Variations in nurse plasma cortisol

Fig. 4: Variations in the subjective alertness of nurses

In bright light, the amount of melatonin secretion decreases and the body temperature increases.

Locomotive activities or sleep can affect many circadian factors such as temperature and melatonin, but cortisol secretion is not controlled by acute changes in locomotion. So this research indicates that the rate of change in cortisol secretion in normal lighting and in bright lighting is exactly the same (Fig. 2).

The comparison of Fig. 2 and 3 show that the amount of melatonin secretion (in both normal lighting and bright lighting) is maximum at 4 am, while the amount of cortisol secretion in the body at this time is minimum. It is obvious that the model for concentration changes of melatonin and cortisol is opposite.

The maximum consciousness rate is in the initial work hours (7 pm in night work) and the minimum consciousness rate is identical to the 5 am rate. Rate of consciousness does not change in bright light and normal light (Fig. 4). This is maybe because of duration’s time of bright light exposure and it is need more research. After a night work if nurses have sufficient rest, the rate of consciousness will increase in the afternoon. The present investigation indicates that although bright light is effective on melatonin secretion, but this change does not affect the rate of consciousness and fatigue during the day (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5: Variations in nurse fatigue

The comparison between diagrams 4 and 1 shows, we would have the least consciousness rate when the temperature is the lowest as well.

DISCUSSION

Until recently, chronobiologists agreed that social cues were the main zeitgebers for human circadian rhythms and that the light-dark cycle, which is the most important zeitgeber for animals, was relatively unimportant for humans (Wever et al., 1983). This view changed radically, however, after the discovery that exposure to bright light suppresses nighttime melatonin production (Lewy and Newsome, 1983), whereas ordinary room light is not sufficiently intense to be effective (Lewy et al., 1980). One implication of these findings was that exposure to sunlight, which is generally 20 to 200 times as bright as indoor light, could synchronize human biological rhythms that remain unaffected by indoor light. Their study indicates that the bright light (4500 lux) exposure can suppress melatonin production. A second implication was that bright artificial light could be used experimentally and perhaps therapeutically, to manipulate biological rhythms in humans. In the present investigation, the idea was examined again and the same results were obtained.

Exposure to bright light increased nocturnal body temperature level and improved performance when examined under controlled constant-routine conditions (Badia et al., 1991; Wright et al., 1997a, b; Foret et al., 1998).

Also many studies said exposure to several hours of bright light for up to 3 consecutive days has been found to reset the endogenous circadian rhythms of core body temperature, cortisol, melatonin, alertness, performance (Allan and Czeisler, 1994; Van Cauter et al., 1994; Czeisler, 1995; Dawson et al., 1995; Eastman et al., 1995; Duffy et al., 1996; Shanahan et al., 1997; Youngstedt et al., 2002) that we obtained in this study as well (Fig. 1-5). Due to the mentioned results, when nurses who are participating in this research are in contact with bright light, their body temperature is at the lowest amount at 5 a.m., while in normal lighting conditions, the minimum body temperature is detectable in 3 to 5 a.m. As a result, this delayed temperature phase could be termed a response to the bright light exposure. These results were also obtained from other researches as well.

Studies been shown in animal studies that exposure to brief pulses of bright light can phase shift the circadian pacemaker and that the resetting action of light is most efficient during the first minutes of light exposure. Present research also proved that contacted with bright light for 45 min can shift the circadian pacemaker.

In humans, contact with bright light for consecutive days can cause the delay phase. For instance it is proved that the bright light contact for three consecutive days (40 to 60 min per day) can cause the delay phase in the circadian rhythm (Baehr et al., 2000).

The ability of the hormone melatonin to shift the circadian clock makes it a promising potential treatment for circadian maladaptation problems arising from jet lag and shift work. Our study demonstrated that the nurses in contact with bright light didn’t have a significant increase in alertness.

The human PRC to melatonin is apparently 180° out of phase with the PRC to light (Lewy et al.,1998), so that melatonin administered before CBT min will advance the clock. In this research the obtained results are the same.

In the Kenneth’s analysis of high-low body temperature performance, he found that a higher average body temperature of only ~0.15°C was associated with higher performance, suggesting that small changes in body temperature can influence human performance (Kenneth et al., 2002).

CONCLUSION

This study indicated that when the temperature is the lowest (between 3-5 a.m.), alertness rate is the least amount (Fig. 4) and fatigue increased in these hours (Fig. 5). Another outcome of this research is that intermittent bright light can facilitate circadian rhythm adaptation to the night shift. Further research should investigate the phase-shifting effects of even shorter durations for real-world applications.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

The authors would like to thank Shiraz and Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Namazie Hormons Research Center and Namazie’s hospital nurses.

REFERENCES

  • Allan, J.S. and C.A. Czeisler, 1994. Persistence of the circadian thyrotropin rhythm under constant conditions and after light-induced shifts of circadian phase. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab., 79: 508-512.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Badia, P., B. Myers, M. Boecker, J. Culpepper and J.R. Harsh, 1991. Bright light effects on body temperature, alertness, EEG and behavior. Physiol. Behav., 50: 583-588.
    CrossRef    


  • Baehr, E.K., W. Revelle and C.I. Eastman, 2000. Individual difference in the phase amplitude of the human circadian temperature rhythm: With an emphasis on morningness-eveningness. J. Sleep Res., 9: 117-127.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Bonnet, M.H., 1990. Dealing with shift work: Physical fitness, temperature and napping. Work Stress, 4: 261-274.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    


  • Brown-DeGagne, A.M. and G.A. Eskes, 1998. Turning body time to shift time. Can. Nurse, 94: 51-52.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Cajochen, C., S.B.S. Khalsa, J.K. Wyatt, C.A. Czeisler and D.J. Dijk, 1999. EEG and ocular correlates of circadian melatonin phase and human performance decrements during sleep loss. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol., 277: 640-649.
    Direct Link    


  • Czeisler, C.A., 1995. The effect of light on the human circadian pacemaker. Ciba Found Symp., 183: 254-290.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Dawson, D., N. Encel and K. Lushington, 1995. Improving adaptation to simulated night shift: Timed exposure to bright light versus daytime melatonin administration. Sleep, 18: 11-21.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Dijk, D.J., J.F. Duffy and C.A. Czeisler, 1992. Circadian and sleep/wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive performance. J. Sleep Res., 1: 112-117.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Dinges, D.F., F. Pack, K. Williams, K.A. Gillen and J.W. Powell et al., 1997. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. Sleep, 20: 267-277.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Duffy, J.F., R.F. Kronauer and C.A. Czeisler, 1996. Phase-shifting human circadian rhythms: Influence of sleep timing, social contact and light exposure. J. Physiol., 495: 289-297.
    Direct Link    


  • Eastman, C.I., L. Liu and L.F. Fogg, 1995. Circadian rhythm adaptation to simulated night shift work: Effect of nocturnal bright-light duration. Sleep, 18: 399-407.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Estryn-Behar, M., M. Kaminski, F. Peigne, N. Bonnet and E. Vaichere et al., 1990. Stress at work and mental health status among hospital workers. Br. J. Ind. Med., 47: 20-28.
    CrossRef    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Foret, J., A. Daurat and G. Tirilly, 1998. Effect of bright light at night on core temperature, subjective alertness and performance as a function of exposure time. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health, 3: 115-120.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Goldberg, D. and V.C. Hillier, 1979. A scaled version of the general health questionnaire. Psychol. Med., 9: 139-145.
    CrossRef    


  • Hawley, M.P., 1992. Sources of stress for emergency nurses in four urban Canadian emergency departments. J. Emerg. Nurs., 18: 211-216.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Hunt, T. and P. Sassone-Corsi, 2007. Riding tandem: Circadian clocks and the cell cycle. Cell, 129: 461-464.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    


  • Jaffe, M.P., M.H. Smolensky and C.C. Wun, 1996. Sleep quality and physical and social well being in North American petrochemical shift workers. South Med. J., 89: 305-312.
    Direct Link    


  • Kenneth, P., J., Wright, J.T. Hull and C.A. Czeisler, 2002. Relationship between alertness, performance and temperature in humans. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integrative Comp. Physiol., 283: R1370-R1377.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Khalsa, S.B.S., E.J. Megan, C. Christian and C.A. Czeisler, 2003. A phase response curve to single bright light pulses in human subjects. J. Physiol., 549: 945-952.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    


  • Lewy, A. and D. Newsome, 1983. Different types of melatonin circadian secretory rhythms in some blind subjects. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab., 56: 1103-1107.
    Direct Link    


  • Lewy, A., T. Wehr and F. Goodwin, 1980. Light suppresses melatonin secretion in humans. Science, 210: 1267-1269.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Lewy, A., S. Ahmed and R.L. Sack, 1995. Phase shifting the human circadian clock using melatonin. Behav. Brain Res., 73: 131-134.
    CrossRef    


  • Lewy, A., V.K. Bauer, S. Ahmed, K.H. Thomas and N.L. Cutler et al., 1998. The human phase response curve (PRC) to melatonin is about 12 hours out of phase with the PRC to light. Chronobiol. Int., 15: 71-83.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Nemchin, A.A., M.J. Whitehouse, M. Menneken, T. Geisler, R.T. Pidgeon and S.A. Wilde, 2008. A light carbon reservoir recorded in zircon-hosted diamond from the Jack Hills. Nature, 454: 92-95.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    


  • Saiepour, N., K. Mohammad, R. Abhari, H. Zeraati and A.A. Noorbala, 2008. Mental disorder assessed by general health questionnaire and back pain among postmenopausal Iranian women. Pak. J. Biol. Sci., 11: 809-812.
    CrossRef    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Scott, A.J., 1994. Chronobiological considerations in shift worker sleep and performance and shift work scheduling. Hum. Performance, 7: 207-233.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    


  • Shanahan, T.L., J.M. Zeitzer and C.A. Czeisler, 1997. Resetting the melatonin rhythm with light in humans. J. Biol. Rhythms, 12: 556-567.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Stevens, R.G., D.E. Blask, G.C. Brainard, J. Hansen and S.W. Lockley et al., 2007. Meeting report: The role of environmental lighting and circadian disruption in cancer and other diseases. Environ. Health Perspectives, 115: 1357-1362.
    Direct Link    


  • Van Cauter, E., J. Sturis, M.M. Byrne, J.D. Blackman and R. Leproult et al., 1994. Demonstration of rapid light-induced advances and delays of the human circadian clock using hormonal phase markers. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab., 266: E953-E963.
    Direct Link    


  • Vujovic, N., A.J. Davidson and M. Menaker, 2008. Sympathetic input modulates, but does not determine, phase of peripheral circadian oscillators. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integrative Comp. Physiol., 295: R355-R360.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Wever, R., J. Polasek and C. Wildgruber, 1983. Bright light affects human circadian rhythms. Pflugers Arch., 396: 85-87.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    


  • Wright, K.P.J, P. Badia, B.L. Myers and S.C. Plenzler, 1997. Combination of bright light and caffeine as a countermeasure for impaired alertness and performance during extended sleep deprivation. J. Sleep Res., 6: 26-35.
    Direct Link    


  • Wright, K.P.J., P. Badia, B.L. Myers, S.C. Plenzler and M. Hakel, 1997. Caffeine and light effects on nighttime melatonin and temperature levels in sleep-deprived humans. Brain Res., 747: 78-84.
    CrossRef    


  • Wyatt, J.K., A.R. de Cecco, C.A. Czeisler and D.J. Dijk, 1999. Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integrative Comp. Physiol., 277: R1152-R1163.
    Direct Link    


  • Youngstedt, S.D., D.F. Kripke and J.A. Elliott, 2002. Circadian phase-delay effects of bright light alone and combined with exercise in humans. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integrative Comp. Physiol., 282: R259-R266.
    PubMed    Direct Link    


  • Ardekani, Z.Z., H. Kakooei, S.M.T. Ayattollahi, A. Choobineh and G.N. Seraji, 2008. Prevalence of mental disorders among shift work hospital nurses in Shiraz, Iran. Pak. J. Biol. Sci., 11: 1605-1609.
    CrossRef    PubMed    Direct Link    

  • © Science Alert. All Rights Reserved