HOME JOURNALS CONTACT

Journal of Applied Sciences

Year: 2008 | Volume: 8 | Issue: 24 | Page No.: 4698-4701
DOI: 10.3923/jas.2008.4698.4701
Gender and Grade Level Differences in Epistemological Beliefs of Iranian Undergraduate Students
R. Marzooghi, M. Fouladchang and B. Shemshiri

Abstract: The aim of this study was to investigate gender and grade level differences in epistemological beliefs of undergraduate students in an Iranian university. The sample consisted of 302 Iranian students at Shiraz University (Mean age = 20.78 year, SD = 1.58), selected by random clustering sampling. They completed Epistemological Beliefs Questionnaire. Results showed males had some more naïve epistemological beliefs than females. Also, first year students viewed learning as quick and knowledge as simple more than last year students.

Fulltext PDF Fulltext HTML

How to cite this article
R. Marzooghi, M. Fouladchang and B. Shemshiri, 2008. Gender and Grade Level Differences in Epistemological Beliefs of Iranian Undergraduate Students. Journal of Applied Sciences, 8: 4698-4701.

Keywords: fast learning, simple knowledge, innate ability, Ability to learn and certainty of knowledge

INTRODUCTION

Epistemological beliefs which defined as individual`s beliefs about knowledge and knowing (Pintrich, 2002) have been first introduced in educational research about 30 years ago (Perry, 1970). Todays, these beliefs have received a considerable amounts of attention from researchers (Phan, 2008; Lodewyk, 2007; Cano, 2005; Whitmire, 2004; Schommer, 1993a). Schommer (1990) proposed a multidimensional system of epistemological beliefs as opposed to unidimentional one. Schommer hypothesized five dimensions of epistemological beliefs including.

Stability (Is knowledge certain or tentative?), structure (Is knowledge simple or complex? Is it isolated or integrated?), source (Does knowledge originate from an external source or from personal experience?), speed of acquisition (Is knowledge acquired quickly or gradually? Is the process easy or effortful?) and control of acquisition (Is the ability to learn fixed or developed overtime?).

To assess these different dimensions of epistemological beliefs, Schommer (1990) developed an instrument and found evidence to support the multidimensionality of beliefs. Furthermore, Schommer (1990) argued that these beliefs were more or less independent from one another.

Schommer (1993a) reports that the higher the beliefs in innate learning, simple knowledge, quick learning and certainty of knowledge, the higher the student`s tendency to subscribe to a simpler view of the world. Schommer`s research caused a lot of interest on epistemological beliefs and metacognition in learning processes and produced a series of research on related issues such as epistemological beliefs in text comprehension (Schommer et al., 1992), goals and academic performance among students (Braten and Stromso, 2004; Schommer, 1993b), comparison of beliefs between gifted and non-gifted high school students (Marzooghi, 1996; Schommer and Dunnell, 1994) and domain independentness of epistemological beliefs (Schommer and Kiersten, 1995).

There have also been a series of research focused on gender differences in epistemological beliefs, but, their results are inconclusive. Some studies have found important gender differences in epistemological beliefs (Cano, 2005; Baxter Magolda, 1992; Belenky et al., 1986). In some studies, females showed more advanced beliefs than (Lodewyk, 2007; Mason et al., 2006; Schommer and Dunnell, 1994; Schommer, 1993a). On the other hands, there are many other studies that find almost no gender differences in epistemological thinking or beliefs (Phan, 2008a; Buehl et al., 2002; Hofer, 2006; Kuhn et al., 2000; King and Kitchener, 1994; Kuhn, 1991).

Pintrich (2002) has suggested that there may not be important gender differences in epistemological thinking when it is defined in terms of separate dimensions of epistemological beliefs. That is, when individuals are asked to focus on specific dimensions of epistemological beliefs, rather than more holistic and general ways of thinking, gender differences do not emerge.

Development of epistemological beliefs during academic years has also been studied. Some researchers found evidence in supporting the effect of grade level on epistemological beliefs of students in middle school or university years (Mason et al., 2006; Cano, 2005; Kuhn et al., 2000; King and Kitchener, 1994; Schommer, 1993a; Jehng et al., 1993; Perry, 1970). Mason et al. (2006) noted at the different ages, more educated students hold more sophisticated beliefs about the nature and acquisition of knowledge. University students with more sophisticated epistemological beliefs use more elaborative learning strategies, enjoy thinking deeply about subject and are motivated to learn because of an inherent interest in or appreciation for the subject (Harris, 2003). Research findings showed that epistemological beliefs of undergraduates affect topic, the use of mediators, search techniques, the evaluation of information and the ability to recognize authority (Whitmire, 2003, 2004).

In a longitudinal study, Phan (2008b) showed that epistemological beliefs influenced students learning approaches. Braten and Stromso (2005) noted that epistemological beliefs predict self- regulated learning among Norwegian postsecondary students. Also research findings suggest that higher education institutions should provide scaffolding to foster the development of a mature learning experiences among their students (Rodrigues and Cano, 2006).

The purpose of the present study was to investigate gender and grade level differences in epistemological beliefs among Iranian undergraduate students. This provides the opportunity to determine whether gender and grade level differences were related to differences in students` epistemological beliefs across a different culture (Iran). Two questions of this research are ; Is there any significant differences between epistemological beliefs of male and female and between first and last year students in a sample of Iranian undergraduate students?

MATERIALS AND METHODS

The study was carried out in 2007-2008 education year. The participants were 302 Iranian undergraduate students at Shiraz University (64% female and 36% male), selected by random clustering sampling. They consisted of students from the faculties of Humanities (16%), Education and Psychology (38%), Art (7%) and Engineering (39%). The age range of the respondents were between 18 to 27 years (Mean = 20.78, SD = 1.58). Most students came from middle income families, 51% of them were 1st year and 49% were 4th year students.

Instrument: Schommer`s (1990) Epistemological Questionnaire has served as an effective tool to assess student`s beliefs regarding learning and the nature of knowledge. Schommer`s questionnaire has 63 items divided into four subscales of fixed ability, simple knowledge, fast learning and certainty of knowledge. Items scored on a 5 point likert-type scale, with 1 = strongly disagree and 5 strongly agree for each item.

The questionnaire has been adapted by Marzooghi (1996) in a earlier study on Iranian students. Marzooghi (1996) did a factor analysis on the data and extracted five factors including:

Inability to learn to learn
Innate ability
Fast learning
Simple learning
Certainty of knowledge

This adapted instrument was used for data gathering in this study and Cronbach`s alpha coefficients were calculated as a measure of the reliability of the data obtained. Alphas` coefficients were from 0.52 to 0.63. In Schommer`s studies, the reliability coefficients were in the range of 0.45 to 0.71 (Schommer, 1993a).

Also, items having low correlation with subscales` total scores were deleted. As a results, 51 items remained in the questionnaire. Mean composite scores for each of the variables were used in the current investigation.

Procedure: The student`s agreement to participate in the study was sought. The students agreed and completed the questionnaire during classroom time. They were asked to give other information such as their age, grade level and sex.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

To study gender differences in epistemological beliefs, a multi variate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted, with the dimensions of epistemological beliefs as dependent variables and gender as independent variable (Table 1). The analysis yielded significant differences between male and female students on the beliefs of ability to learn how to learn [F (1, 280) = 7.83, p<0.005], innate ability [F (1, 280) = 5.78, p<0.017] and simple learning [F (1, 280) = 7.83, p<0.005] (Table 2).

Table 1: Means and standard deviations for females and males on epistemological beliefs scores
The values are shown in Mean ± SD

Table 2: Tests of between subjects effects
SS: Sum of Squares, df: Degree of freedom, MS: Mean Square, ns: Not significant

Table 3: Means and standard deviations for first and last graders on epistemological beliefs scores
The values are shown in Mean ± SD

In these beliefs, mean score of males were higher than females`. It means male students showed naive view of learning significantly more than female students. There was no significant difference between males and females in beliefs of fast learning and certainty of knowledge.

Similar to the previous analysis, regarding grade level differences, a MANOVA was computed, with the dimensions of epistemological beliefs as dependent variables and grade level as independent variable. Descriptive statistics are shown in Table 3.

MANOVA revealed that first year students significantly differentiated from last year students in the beliefs of innate ability [F (1, 300) = 6, p<0.02] and simple learning [F (1, 300) = 5, p<0.03] (Table 4). In these beliefs, mean score of first graders were higher than last graders`. It means innate and simple view of knowledge appears to decrease as the year level goes up. The effect of year level on other beliefs scores were not significant.

There was evidence of gender differences in epistemological beliefs of undergraduates. In line with much of the research on gender differences in epistemological beliefs (Lodewyk, 2007; Mason et al., 2006; Cano, 2005; Schommer and Dunnell, 1994; Baxter Magolda, 1992; Belenky et al., 1986) girls were higher than boys in terms of their thinking about the nature of learning, the source of knowledge, or the structure of knowledge.

In addition, the results related to grade level showed last graders had more sophisticated epistemological beliefs than first graders. This provides support for some studies which document a positive trend across graders in middle school or university years (Kuhn et al., 2000; Mason et al., 2006; King and Kitchener, 1994; Schommer, 1993; Jehng et al., 1993; Perry, 1970).

Table 4: Tests of between subjects effects
SS: Sum of Squares, df: Degree of freedom, MS: Mean Square, ns: Not significant

These results have both theoretical and practical values. These findings can help teachers and planners understand how gender and grade level affect individuals` epistemological beliefs. Teachers and classrooms can influence the development of undergraduates` epistemological beliefs which influence the decisions they make as they study and learn. So, there is a need to help students move from naïve epistemologies to more complex epistemological beliefs as Lee and Ashby (2000) suggest.

This research represents an investigation on the gender differences in epistemological beliefs of a nonwestern culture (Iran). Research on similar samples is required to verify the present findings. It is also recommended that future research consider the interaction of gender and other personal or contextual variables, such as age, classroom environment and academic disciplines. In addition, longitudinal studies are needed to determine how epistemological beliefs of students change during university years.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research is sponsored in part by Shiraz University (Our grant No. in Shiraz University is 86-GR-EDU-17).

REFERENCES

  • Baxter Magolda, M.B., 1992. Knowing and Reasoning in College: Gender-related Patterns in Students' Intellectual Development. 1st Edn., Jossey Bass, San Francisco


  • Belenky, M.F., B.M. Clinchy, N.R. Goldberger and J.M. Tarule, 1986. Women's Ways of Knowing: The Development of Self, Voice and Mind. 1st Edn., Basic Books, New York


  • Braten, I. and H.I. Stromso, 2004. Epistemological beliefs and implicit theories of intelligence as predictors of achievement goals. Contemporary Edu. Psychol., 29: 371-388.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    


  • Braten, I. and H.I. Stromso, 2005. The relationship between epistemological beliefs, implicit theories of intelligence and self- regulated learning among Norwegian postsecondary student. Br. J. Edu. Psychol., 75: 539-567.
    Direct Link    


  • Cano, F., 2005. Epistemological beliefs and approaches to learning: Their changes through secondary school and their influence on academic performance. Br. J. Educ. Psychol., 75: 203-221.
    CrossRef    


  • Buehl, M.M., P.A. Alexander and P.K. Murphy, 2002. Beliefs about schooled knowledge: Domain general or domain specific. Contemporary Edu. Psychol., 27: 415-449.
    Direct Link    


  • Harris, C.L., 2003. Understanding the Role of Epistemological Beliefs in Post-Graduate Students: Motivation and Conceptions of Learning in First-Year Law Students. 1st Edn., Publication Dissertation, The University of Texas, Austin


  • Hofer, B.K., 2006. Domain specificity of personal epistemology: Resolved questions, persistent issues, new models psychology department. Int. J. Edu. Res., 45: 85-95.
    CrossRef    


  • Jehng, J.C.J., S.D. Johnson and R.C. Anderson, 1993. Schooling and students' epistemological beliefs about learning. Contemp. Educ. Psychol., 18: 23-35.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    


  • King, P.M. and K.S. Kitchener, 1994. Developing Reflective Judgment. 1st Edn., Jossey-Bass, San Francisco


  • Kuhn, D., 1991. The Skills of Argument. 1st Edn., Cambridge University Press, New York, ISBN-10: 052142349X


  • Kuhn, D., R. Cheney and M. Weinstock, 2000. The development of epistemological understanding. Cognitive Developm., 15: 309-328.
    CrossRef    


  • Lee, P. and R. Ashby, 2000. Progression in Historical Understanding Among Students Ages 7-14. In: Knowing, Teaching, and Learning History: National and International Perspectives, Stearns, P., P. Seixas and S. Wineburg (Eds.). New York University Press, New York, pp: 199-222
    Direct Link    


  • Lodewyk, K.R., 2007. Relations among epistemological beliefs, academic achievement, and task performance in secondary school students. Educ. Psychol.: Int. J. Exp. Educ. Psychol., 27: 307-327.
    CrossRef    


  • Mason, N., A. Boldrin and G. Zurlo, 2006. Epistemological understanding in different judgment domains: Relationship with gender, grade level and Curriculum. Int. J. Edu. Res., 45: 43-56.
    CrossRef    


  • Marzooghi, R., 1996. A Comparison of Epistemological Beliefs Between Between Gifted and Non-Gifted High School Students in Karaj City. 1st Edn., Tehran University, Karaj


  • Perry, Jr. W.G., 1970. Forms of Intellectual and Ethical Development in the College Years. 1st Edn., Academic Press, New York


  • Phan, H.P., 2008. Multiple regression analysis of epistemological beliefs, learning approaches, and self-regulated learning. Elect. J. Res. Edu. Psychol., 6: 157-184.
    Direct Link    


  • Phan, H.P., 2008. Predicting change in epistemological beliefs, reflective thinking and Learning styles: A longitudinal study. Br. J. Edu. Psychol., 78: 75-93.
    Direct Link    


  • Pintrich, P.R., 2002. Future Challenges and Directions for the Theory and Research on Personal Epistemology. In: Personal Epistemology: The Psychology of Beliefs about Knowledge and Knowing, Hofer, B.K. and P.R. Pintrich (Eds.). Hillsdale, Erlbaum pp: 389-414


  • Rodrigues, L. and F. Cano, 2006. The epistemological beliefs, learning approaches and study orchestrations of university students. Stud. Higher Edu., 31: 617-636.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    


  • Schommer, M., 1993. Comparisons of beliefs about the nature of knowledge and learning among postsecondary students. Res. Higher Edu., 34: 355-370.
    CrossRef    


  • Schommer, M., 1993. Epistemological development and academicperformance among secondary students. J. Edu. Psychol., 85: 406-411.
    CrossRef    


  • Schommer, M., 1990. Effects of beliefs about the nature of knowledge on comprehension. J. Educ. Psychol., 82: 498-504.
    CrossRef    


  • Schommer, M. and P.A. Dunnell, 1994. A comparison of epistemological beliefs between gifted and non-gifted high school students. Roeper Rev., 16: 207-210.
    CrossRef    


  • Schommer, M., A. Crouse and N. Rhodes, 1992. Epistemological beliefs and mathematical text comprehension: Believing it is simple does not make it so. J. Educ. Psychol., 84: 435-443.
    Direct Link    


  • Schommer, M. and W. Kiersten, 1995. Are epistemological beliefs similar across domains. J. Edu. Psychol., 87: 424-432.
    CrossRef    


  • Whitmire, E., 2003. Epistemological beliefs and information-seeking behavior of undergraduates. Library Inform. Sci. Res., 25: 127-142.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    


  • Whitmire, E., 2004. The relationship between undergraduates' epistemological beliefs, reflective judgment, and their information- seeking behavior. Inform. Process. Manage. Int. J., 40: 97-111.
    CrossRef    Direct Link    

  • © Science Alert. All Rights Reserved