|
|
|
|
Research Article
|
|
Applied Shared Log-Normal Frailty Cox-Proportional Hazard Model to Evaluating the Effect of Vitamin A on the Rat Passive Avoidance Memory |
|
Mohammad Reza Zadkarami
|
|
|
ABSTRACT
|
In this research, the Cox-proportional hazard model is used to investigate
the effect of various values of vitamin A (3000, 4500 and 6000 IU kg-1)
and sesame oil on the passive avoidance memory of rats by shuttle box.
Present results confirm that various values of vitamin A do not improve
the passive avoidance memory of rats (p<0.05). We found that the animals
are clustered (p<0.001) and applying shared log-normal frailty for
clustering improves present results (p<0.05) such that sesame oil improves
the passive avoidance memory task (p<0.05). Therefore we should consider
clustering in the analysis of biological data or we should use cloned
animals.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
INTRODUCTION
In many biostatistics studies, it is often desired to relate risk
factors to the occurrence of a certain event. The Cox-proportional hazard
model is widely applicable in the analysis of such data (Turcani and Rupp,
2000; Song et al., 2003; Katz et al., 2004; Costacou et
al., 2006). Moreover, subject often are clustered and the survival
times within a cluster tend to be correlated (Lin, 1993; Claus et al.,
1998; Bouhali et al., 2007; Weycker et al., 2007).
Clustering is an interesting area in the modeling of medical and biological
experimental data where the subjects of study are often humans or animals.
It is usual to select homogenous group of animals according to the standard
protocols. But the animals usually are clustered because of unobserved
biological variations between the animals or unmeasurable genetic factors
lying behind the life process. However, in biological contexts, heterogeneity
across subjects has usually been ignored (Grodstein et al., 2003;
Vakili et al., 2004; Kumar and Gupta, 2002a; Greenwood et al.,
2007). It is important to include this heterogeneity in the statistical
model in order to avoid misleading results.
Frailty model is useful to handling the within-cluster dependence (Vu
and Knuiman, 2002; Kauermann et al., 2008). In this model assumes
that all the subjects within a cluster share a common unobserved characteristic,
hence the hazard functions for the subjects within a cluster include a
common frailty (Hougaard, 2000; Chuang et al., 2005; Pankratz et
al., 2005) and Log-normal frailty model is one of popular frailty
density function when using Cox-proportional hazard model (Hougaard, 2000;
Kalbfleisch and Prentice, 2002; Vu, 2004).
Vitamin A is fat-soluble nutrients that exert various important roles
especially during the early stages of life (Bates, 1983; Basu and Dickerson,
1996; Clagett-Dame and DeLuca, 2002; Berger et al., 2008). Vitamin
A is also fundamental for growth and development and it is essential to
ensure a good functioning of the immune system of the young (Iwata et
al., 2004; Debier et al., 2005). Moreover, vitamin A deficiency
produce spatial learning and memory impairment (Cocco et al., 2002;
Etchamendy et al., 2003; Hernandez-Pinto et al., 2006; Oliveira
et al., 2007) and motor impairment (Cart et al., 2006).
Sesame oil is one of the vegetables oil that consists unsaturated fatty
acid and phosphatidycholine (lecithin). Therefore it is effective on the
decrease of cholesterol and improve spatial learning and memory in adult
male mice, Moazedi et al. (2002).
Vitamin A (3000, 4500 and 6000 IU kg-1) administered Intra
muscular injection. The animals are injected for 6 days consecutively.
Then all animals tested for a passive avoidance memory task by shuttle
box. These groups have been compared with vitamin A sham group (sesame
oil, 0.5 cc, injected group) and control group (any injection). The data
is analyzed by one-way analysis of variances (ANOVA) (Adal, 2004). In
this research the data reanalysis by using Cox-proportional hazard model
with and without consider animals clustering.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
The young male NMRI rats (100±5 g and one month age) have
been used. Animals were housed at constant temperature 23±2°C
on a 12 h light/dark cycle and food and water available ad libitum.
All experimental protocols were applied in strict accordance with international
guidelines regulating the use of animals for scientific purposes. This
protocol has been done in department of Biology, Shahid Chamran University,
Ahvaz, Iran, in 2004.
All animals tested for a passive avoidance memory task by the shuttle
box, a Plexiglas box with a steel-rod floor (parallel rods, 0.25 cm in
diameter set 0.8 cm a part), which includes two equal 30x20x20 cm connected
parts, light part and dark part. The dark part is covered by Aluminum.
There is a controlled door, 65x8 cm, between light and dark parts which
is opened whenever it is needed. The shuttle box has been used to test
memory activity and passive avoidance in animals (Lipman and Colowick,
1988; Kumar and Gupta, 2002a, b; Vakili et al., 2004; Micale
et al., 2004; Pumo et al., 2006; Greenwood et al.,
2007; Jin et al., 2007).
One day before behavioral test, each rat was individually placed on the
shuttle box and allowed to visit two parts freely for 3 min. After a rat
had visited the box, it was removed from the box and returned to its waiting
cage.
Each rat was gently placed on the light part 24 h later. When the rat
leaves to the light part and placed its paws on the grid floor of dark
part, the time is recorded (pre-test time). After 5 sec, it was taken
an electric shock 1.5 mA (milli Ampere) without intermission for 2 sec.
The rat removed from the box and returned to its home cage after 20 sec.
The rats are clustered by the pre-test time such that the rat is clustered
as an active rat if the pre-test time equal or less than 10 sec, otherwise
it is clustered as an inactive rat.
Memory testing was performed 48 h later, in which each rat was placed
on the light part again and the step into light part time recorded (test-time),
in the absence of electric shock, with a step into time as passive avoidance
behaviour. An upper cut-off of 300 sec was set (censored time). In this
case, test-time was considered as 300 sec. It is cleared that the test
time is not normal distributed and we cannot apply the one-way analysis
of variance (ANOVA) to compare various groups of rats.
Drugs: The following drugs were used for passive memory: Vitamin
A from GMB Co. (Germany), animal food from Pars animal food Co. (Iran).
Rats were purchased from Razi institute (Karaj-Iran) and shuttle box with
shock controlled was provided from Nortab electric Co. (Iran).
Experimental procedure: Thirty five animals were divided randomly
into five groups so that each group includes seven animals. Vitamin A
(3000, 4500 and 6000 IU kg-1) (1 IU = 0.300-0.344 μg),
values administered Intra muscular (im) injection. Vitamin A for 3 days
injected consecutively, the doses were adjusted in a way for each animal
to receive a volume of 0.5 cc by adding sesame oil. These groups have
been compared with vitamin A sham group (sesame oil, 0.5 cc (μL),
injected) and with control group (no injection). All animal were tested,
by the shuttle box, 24 h after last injection.
Data analysis: It is cleared that the normality of the data is
not satisfied. The data is analyzed firstly by the Cox-proportional hazard
model. In second time, the data is analyzed by the Cox-proportional hazard
model with the log-normal frailty. In fact the pre-test time is considered
for effect of the biological variations between the animals or genetic
factors lying behind the life process of animals on the rats memory.
The analysis of the data was done by Splus 2000 statistical package.
However these models are provided by any standard statistical package.
RESULTS
The frequency of observing event (enter into dark part) and censored
(never enter into dark part) for two clusters, active and inactive is
shown in Table 1. We found that there is no statistical
significance between censored percentage of two clusters (p<0.05).
The results of the 95% confidence interval for the pre-test time for
two cultures, active and inactive, are shown in Fig. 1.
The average of pre-test time is 5.103 and 34.266 sec for the active and
inactive clusters, respectively.
Statistical test confirm that the two clusters of rats are significantly
different (p<0.001). Therefore the animals are clustered. Moreover,
Fig. 2 shows the hazard of two clusters of rats, active
and inactive, in fitting the Cox-proportional hazard model. As we can
see the two clusters of animals have different hazard functions.
The results in Table 2 indicate that the sham group
and different values of vitamin A groups did not statistical significance
from control group (p<0.05) in the Cox-proportional hazard model. However,
the sham (sesame oil) and 4500 IU kg-1 value of vitamin A improve
the passive avoidance memory of animals a little, with p-values 0.051
and 0.079, respectively, but there are not statistically significant (p<0.05).
Table 1: |
Frequency of event and censored in the two clusters
of rats |
 |
Table 2: |
Results of fitting Cox-proportional hazard model with
and without the log-normal frailty model |
 |
 |
Fig. 1: |
95% confidence interval for pre-test time for two groups
of rats |
 |
Fig. 2: |
Hazard function at mean of covariates |
Adding the log-normal frailty to the Cox-proportional hazard model improves
the results (p<0.05) such that injected sesame oil (sham group) improves
the passive avoidance memory of animals significantly (p<0.05) as shown
in Table 2.
DISCUSSION
Vitamin A is the generic term for a variety of substances including
retinol, retinoic acid, retinyl esters as well as provitamin A carotenoids
such as β-carotene. Retinol and its derivatives are only found in
animal tissues whereas β-carotene is principally found in plants
(Olson, 1984; Blomhoff, 1994; Basu and Dickerson, 1996). Vitamin A plays
a critical role in vision, reproduction, immunity, cell differentiation
as well as growth and development (Olson, 1984; Blomhoff, 1994; Basu and
Dickerson, 1996; Napoli, 1999). More specifically, it is of utmost importance
to allow successful gestation and proper offspring development (Thompson
et al., 1963; Clagett-Dame and Deluca, 2002). It is also essential
for the development and good functioning of the immune system (Blomhoff
and Smeland, 1994; Semba, 1998). Vitamin A deficiency may induce foetal
resorption, stillbirth and malformation (Bates, 1983; Basu and Dickerson,
1996; Clagett-Dame and DeLuca, 2002). It is also responsible for longer
diseases, retarded growth and higher mortality in young mammals (Semba,
1998).
Experimental data suggest that oxygen free radicals are probably involved
in the deterioration of cognitive processes. Vitamin A is powerful antioxidant
and it protect tissues against the damage of the free radicals (Groff
et al., 1995; Ross, 1999; Bickford et al., 2000; Alfos
et al., 2001). Previous studies have been found that vitamin A
deficiency produce spatial learning and memory impairment in rats (Cocco
et al., 2002) and mice (Etchamendy et al., 2003). It was
also showed that vitamin A deficiency produces a severe deficit in spatial
learning and memory which are linked to a proper hippocampal functioning
(Hernandez-Pinto et al., 2006).
We found that the various values (3000, 4500 and 6000 IU kg-1),
of vitamin A and sesame oil did not improve the passive avoidance of rats
in the Cox-proportional hazard model without the frailty (p<0.05).
However, adding the frailty to the model to handling clustering improve
the results and we can see that sesame oil improves the passive avoidance
memory of rats significantly (p<0.05). As resulted in Fig.
1, 2 and Table 2 showed the animals
are clustered. Ignoring this heterogeneity across animals resulted in
misleading statistical inference. Therefore this heterogeneity of animals
should be considered to avoid misleading results. Otherwise we should
to provide the biological homogeneous groups of animals (or cloned animals).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We wish to thank Mr. M. Adel and his supervisor, Dr. A.A. Moazedi
for providing the data. The author would like to thanks the referees for
their very helpful comments.
|
REFERENCES |
1: Adal, M., 2004. Investigated the effect of vitamins A and C on motor activity and avoidance memory task in young and aged male rats. M.Sc. Thesis. Shahid Chamran University, Ahvaz, Iran.
2: Alfos, S., C. Boucherone and V. Pallet, 2001. A retinoic acid receptor antagonist suppresses brain retinoic acid receptor over expression and reverses a working memory deficit induced by chronic ethanol consumption in mice. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res., 25: 1506-1514. Direct Link |
3: Basu, T.K. and J.W.T. Dickerson, 1996. Vitamin A: CAB International Vitamins in Human Health and Disease. Biddles Ltd., Guildford, UK., pp: 148-177.
4: Bates, C.J., 1983. Vitamin A in pregnancy and lactation. Proc. Nutr. Soc., 42: 65-79. CrossRef | Direct Link |
5: Berger, S.G., S.D. Pee, M.W. Bloem, S. Halati and R.D. Semba, 2008. Malnutrition and morbidity among children not reached by the national vitamin a capsule programme in urban slum areas of Indonesia. Public Health, 122: 371-378. Direct Link |
6: Bickford, P.C., B. Shukitt-hale, J. Joseph, T. Gould and L. Briederick et al., 2000. Antioxidant rich diets improve cerebellar physiology and motor learning. Brain Res., 866: 211-217. Direct Link |
7: Blomhoff, R., 1994. Overview of Vitamin A Metabolism and Function. In: Vitamins in Health and Disease, Blomhoff, R. (Ed.). Marcel Dekker, New York, pp: 1-35.
8: Blomhoff, R. and E.B. Smeland, 1994. Role of Retinoids in Normal Hematopoiesis and the Immune System. In: Vitamins in Health and Disease, Blomhoff, R. (Ed.). Marcel Dekker, New York, pp: 451-485.
9: Bouhali, T., D. Brisson, J. St-Pierre, G. Tremblay, P. Perron, C. Laprise, M. Vohi and D. Gaudet, 2008. Low plasma adiponectin exacerbates the risk of premature coronary artery disease in familial hypercholesterolemia. Atherosclerosis, 196: 262-269. Direct Link |
10: Cart, M., R. Stancampiano, E. Tronci, M. Collu, A. Usiello, M. Morelli and F. Fadda, 2006. Vitamin A deficiency induces motor impairments and striatal cholinergic dysfunction in rats. Neuroscience, 139: 1163-1172. Direct Link |
11: Chuang, S.K., T. Cai, C.W. Douglass, L.J. Wei and T.B. Dodson, 2005. Frailty approach for the analysis of clustered failure time observations in dental research. J. Dent. Res., 84: 54-58. Direct Link |
12: Clagett-Dame, M. and H.F. DeLuca, 2002. The role of vitamin A in mammalian reproduction and embryonic development. Ann. Rev. Nutr., 22: 347-381. Direct Link |
13: Claus, J.J., W.A.V. Gool, S. Teunisse, G.J.M. Walstra and V.I.H. Kwa et al., 1998. Predicting survival in patients with early Alzheimer’s disease. Deme. Geri. Conn. Disor., 9: 284-293.
14: Cocco, S., G. Diaz, R. Stancampiano, A. Diana and M. Carta et al., 2002. Vitamin A deficiency produces a spatial learning and memory impairment in rats. Neuroscience, 115: 475-482. Direct Link |
15: Costacou, T., Y. Chang, R.E. Ferrell and T.J. Orchard, 2006. Identifying genetic susceptibilities to diabetes-related complications among individuals at low risk of complications: An application of tree-structured survival analysis. Am. J. Epid., 164: 862-872. Direct Link |
16: Debier, C., J. Pottier, C. Goffe and Y. Larondelle, 2005. Present knowledge and unexpected behaviours of vitamins A and E in Colostrum and milk. Livestock Prod. Sci., 98: 135-147. Direct Link |
17: Etchamendy, N., V. Enderlin, A. Marighetto, V. Pallet, P. Higueret and R. Jaffard, 2003. Vitamin A deficiency and relational memory deficit in adult mice: Relationship with changes in brain retinoid signaling. Behav. Brain Res., 145: 37-49. Direct Link |
18: Greenwood, B., P.V. Strong, A.A. Dorey and M. Fleshner, 2007. Wheel running reverses stress-induced interference with shuttle box escape. Behav. Neurosci., 121: 992-1000. Direct Link |
19: Grodstein, F., J. Chen and W.C. Willett, 2003. High-dose antioxidant supplements and cognitive function in community-dwelling elderly. Am. J. Clin. Nutr., 77: 975-984. Direct Link |
20: Groff, J.L., S.S. Gropper and S.M. Hunt, 1995. The Fat Soluble Vitamins: Advanced Nutrition and Human Metabolism. West Publishing Company, Minneapolis, pp: 284-324.
21: Hernandez-Pinto, A.M., L. Puebla-Jimenez and E. Arilla-Ferreiro, 2006. A vitamin A-free diet results in impairment of the rat hippocampal somatostatinergic system. Neuroscience, 141: 851-861. CrossRef | PubMed | Direct Link |
22: Hougaard, P., 2000. Analysis of Multivariate Survival Data. Springer-Verlag, New York, USA., ISBN: 978-1-4612-7087-4, Pages: 542.
23: Iwata, M., A. Hirakiyama, Y. Eshima, H. Kagechika, C. Kato and S.Y. Song, 2004. Retinoic acid imprints gut-homing specificity on T cells. Immunity, 21: 527-538. Direct Link |
24: Jin, U.H., J.H. Kim, G.T. Chang, J.K. Kim, K.H. Chung and C.H. Kim, 2007. Enhancement of learning and memory by a medicinal formulation, saenhyetang, in mice. J. Ethnoph., 109: 271-280. Direct Link |
25: Kalbfleisch, J.D. and R.C. Prentice, 2002. The Statistical Analysis of Failure Time Data. 2nd Edn., John Wiley and Sons, New Jersey.
26: Katz, K.D., S.C. Curry, D.E. Brooks and R.D. Gerkin, 2004. The effect of cyclosporine A on survival time in salicylate-poisoned rats. J. Am. Med., 26: 151-155. Direct Link |
27: Kauermann, G., R. Xu and F. Vaida, 2008. . Stacked Laplace-EM algorithm for duration models with time-varying and random effects. Comput. Stat. Data Anal., 52: 2514-2528. Direct Link |
28: Kumar, M.H.V. and Y.K. Gupta, 2002. Effect of different extracts of Centella asiatica on cognition and markers of oxidative stress in rates. J. Ethnopharm., 79: 253-260. Direct Link |
29: Kumar, M.H.V. and Y.K. Gupta, 2002. Antioxidant property of Celastrus panicultus willd: A possible mechanism in enhancing cognition. Phytomedicine, 9: 302-311. Direct Link |
30: Lin, D.Y., 1993. MULCOX2: A general computer program for the Cox regression analysis of multivariate failure time data. Comput. Meth. Prog. Biomed., 40: 279-293. CrossRef | Direct Link |
31: Lipman, J.J., S.P. Colowick, P.L. Lawrence and N.N. Abumrad, 1988. Aluminum induced encephalopathy in the rat. Life Sci., 42: 863-875. PubMed |
32: Micale, V., G.M. Leggio, C. Mazzola and F. Drago, 2004. Cognitive effects of SL65.0155, a serotonin 5-HT4 receptor partial agonist, in animal models of amnesia. Brian Res., 1121: 207-215. CrossRef | Direct Link |
33: Moazedi, A.A., R. Hovayda and A. Rasekh, 2002. The effect of intrahippocampal Ca1 area injection sesame oil on spatial learning and memory in adult male rats. Proceedings of Science Conference 2002, Taiz, Yemen.
34: Napoli, J.L., 1999. Interactions of retinoid binding proteins and enzymes in retinoid metabolism. Biochim. Biophys. Acta, 1440: 139-162. CrossRef | Direct Link |
35: De Oliveira, M.R., R.B. Silvestrin, T.M. Souza and J.C.F. Moreira, 2007. Oxidative stress in the hippocampus, anxiety-like behavior and decreased locomotory and exploratory activity of adult rats: Effect of sub acute vitamin A supplementation at therapeutic doses. Neuro Toxicol., 28: 1191-1199. CrossRef | Direct Link |
36: Olson, J.A., 1984. Vitamin A. In: Nutrition Review’s Present Knowledge in Nutrition, Brown, M.L. (Ed.). The Nutrition Foundation, Washington, D.C., pp: 177-191.
37: Pankratz, V.S., M.D. Andrade and T.M. Therneau, 2005. Random-effect Cox-proportional hazard hazards model: General variance components methods for time-to-event data. Gene. Epid., 28: 97-109. Direct Link |
38: Pumo, R.L., M. Bellia, A. Nicosia, V. Micale and F. Drago, 2006. Long-lasting neurotoxicity of prenatal benzene acute exposure in rats. Toxicology, 223: 227-234. Direct Link |
39: Ross, A.C., 1999. Vitamin A. In: Modern Nutrition in Health and Disease, Shils, M., J. Olson, M. Shike and A.C. Ross (Eds.). Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, pp: 305-313.
40: Semba, R.D., 1998. The role of vitamin A and related retinoids in immune function. Nutr. Rev., 56: S38-S48. Direct Link |
41: Song, J., M. Sakatsume, I. Narita, S. Goto and K. Omori et al., 2003. Peroxisome proliferators-activated receptor YC161T polymorphisms and survival of Japanese patients with immunoglobulin a nephropathy. Clin. Gene, 64: 398-403.
42: Thompson, J.N., J.M.C. Howell and G.A.J. Pitt, 1963. Vitamin A and reproduction in rats. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci., 159: 510-535. CrossRef | Direct Link |
43: Turcani, M. and H. Rupp, 2000. Heart failure development in rats with ascending aortic constriction and angiotensin-converting enzyme inhibition. Br. J. Pharm., 130: 1671-1677. Direct Link |
44: Vakili, A., K. Tayebi, M. R. Jafari, M.R. Zarrindast and B. Djahanguiri 2004. . Effect of ethanol on morphine state-dependent learning in the mouse: Involvement of gabaergic, opioidergic and cholinergic systems. Alcohol. Alcoholism, 39: 427-432. Direct Link |
45: Vu, H.T.V. and M.W. Knuiman, 2002. A hybrid ML-EM algorithm for calculation of maximum likelihood estimates in semiparametric shared frailty models. Comput. Stat. Data Anal., 40: 173-187. Direct Link |
46: Vu, H.T.V., 2004. Estimation in semiparametric conditional shared frailty models with events before study entry. Comput. Stat. Data Anal., 45: 621-637. Direct Link |
47: Weycker, D., G.A. Nichols, M. O’Keeffe-Rosetti, J. Edelsberg, Z.M. Khan, S. Kaura and G. Oster, 2007. Risk-factor clustering and cardiovascular disease risk in hypertensive patients. Am. J. Hyper., 20: 599-607. Direct Link |
|
|
|
 |