|
|
|
|
Research Article
|
|
Comparison of the Effects of Dose-dependent Zinc Chloride on Short-term and Long-term Memory in Young Male Rats |
|
A.A. Moazedi ,
Z. Ghotbeddin
and
G.H. Parham
|
|
|
ABSTRACT
|
The aim of the present study was to evaluate the effects of dose-dependent of zinc chloride on short-term and long-term memory in a shuttle box. Young Wistar rats (94±10 g) (age 27-30 days) consumed zinc chloride drinking water in five different doses (20, 30, 50, 70 and 100 mg kg-1day-1) for two weeks by gavage. After 14 days on experimental diets, a shuttle box used to test short- and long-term memory. Two criteria considering for behavioral test, including latency in entering dark chamber and time spent in the dark chamber. This experiment shows that after 2 weeks oral administration of ZnCl2 with (20, 30 and 50 mg kg-1 day-1) doses, the rats working (short-term) has been improved (p<0.05). Whereas ZnCl2 with 30 mg kg-1 day-1 dose has been more effected than other doses (p<0.001). But rat which received ZnCl2 with 100 mg kg-1 day-1, has been shown significant impairment in working memory (p<0.05) and there was no significant difference in reference (long-term) memory for any of groups. In general, this study has demonstrated that zinc chloride consumption with 30 mg kg-1 day-1 dose for two weeks was more effective than other doses on short-term memory. But consumption of ZnCl2 with 100 mg kg-1 day-1 dose for two week had the negative effect on short-term memory. On the other hand, zinc supplementation did not have an effect on long-term memory.
|
|
|
|
|
INTRODUCTION
The effect of zinc and their supplementation on learning and memory and cognitive
behavior is controversial. So in this research we want to see the effects of
different doses of zinc chloride on the short-term and long-term memory in young
male rats. Several studies have suggested a functional role for zinc in the
mental function. For instance, zinc supplementation improves cognitive behavior
in school-age children (Penland et al., 1997; Maureen et al.,
2004). Zinc has known to be an essential element for more than a hundred years.
To date more than 300 zinc dependent enzymes have been identified in all the
main biochemical pathways. It acts uniquely as a lewis acid catalyst (an electron
acceptor) in all life processes. This metal has been an essential component
of both DNA and RNA polymerases. It is also vital for normal brain development,
particularly concerning the hippocampal function (Takeda et al., 2005,
2004, 2000). Furthermore, as zinc is the most important trace metal in subcellular
DNA and RNA fractions, this will also explain its vital role in the neuronal
maturation and proliferation (Takeda et al., 2005, 2004; Pfeiffer and
Braveman, 2000). At the molecular level, zinc is unevenly distributed in the
brain with the highest concentration in the olfactory bulb and hippocampus (Ono
and Cherian, 1999). In the hippocampus region, zinc participates in neurotransmission.
Glutamatergic vesicles in the mossy fiber region contain ionic zinc, which is
coreleased with glutamate when the neurons are stimulated (Pang and Wang, 1999).
The hippocampal mossy fiber system forms a dense plexus of terminal on apical
dendrites of CA3 pyramidal neurons and is involved in the modulation of processes
related to spatial learning and memory (Tommie, 2000). Zinc is present in most
food, but meat and fish provide the best source, as bioavailability of zinc
from animal products is considered to be far greater than from plant foods.
It is because of plant foods contain high phytic acid and fiber content. Furthermore,
such every day dietary stables as coffee and milk products have been shown to
reduce the bioavailability of zinc in human, because both iron and calcium have
been found to interfere with zinc absorption (Tuula, 1995). In vitro studies
indicate that the GABA (γ-amino butyric acid) and NMDA (N-methyl-D-aspartate)
receptor, which participate in memory formation, are modulated by zinc (Patenaude
et al., 2002; Li et al., 2001; Chu et al., 2003). Many
investigators have evaluated the association between fetal zinc nutritious and
brain development in early life and established a negative effect of prenatal
zinc deficiency on the brain function of experimental animals. Adverse consequences
include reduced activity and responsiveness; impaired learning ability, attention
and memory (Tuula, 1995).
Because of zinc is an essential dietary element associated with cognition and deficiency of this vital element is able to impair seriously in brain size, total brain count and learning and memory function, so the present study was conducted to test the effects of different doses of zinc supplementation on short-term and long-term memory in young male rats and find out the effective and adverse dose of this vital element on cognitive behaviors. MATERIALS AND METHODS Animals: Were 60 male Wistar rats (94±10), 27-31 days old, were individually housed in stainless cages at a temperature of 23±2°C and a 12/12 h cycle: 7:00 am light on, 7:00 pm light off. All animals were provided from Ahvaz University of Medical Science animal house. Then they were divided into six groups. One group was control group with free access to food and water and five groups drank zinc chloride in different doses (20, 30, 50, 70, 100 mg kg-1 day-1) by gavage methods for two weeks. The amount of time required for this study was 6 months. All experiments were performed in the laboratory of learning and memory in biology department of Shahid Chamran University in Iran. Apparatus: The apparatus used for passive avoidance response training was shuttle box, which consisted of two adjacent Plexiglas compartments of identical dimensions (27x14.5x14) cm. Two compartments separated by a sliding door in the middle part of this apparatus. Of the two compartments, one is illuminated and the other is dark. A sliding door separated the two compartments and could be lowered to form a 2.5 cm hurdle. The floor consisted of 6 mm diameter stainless-steel rods spaced 1.7 cm between centers. The rods were connected to shock generator which could deliver to either compartment a scrambled foot shock, a flashing light (7.5 W) was fixed to the outside wall of the white chamber. Procedure: The first day (Acquisition) rats had free access to either the light or dark compartment of the box, on the second day (Training) rat was placed in the illuminated compartment and 30 sec later the sliding door was raised. Upon entering the dark compartment the door was closed and a 1.5 mA constant-current shock was applied for 2 sec after 20 sec the rat was removed from the dark compartment and placed into home cage. For testing short-term and long-term memory, 48 h (two days) and one month (30 days) after passive avoidance response training, the rat was placed in illuminated chamber and 30 sec later the sliding door was raised and the latency of entering the dark compartment (step-through latency) and the time spent there during 5 min was recorded because the maximum time that considered for this procedure were 300 sec (5 min). (Arlene et al., 1997; Takeda et al., 2005). Data analysis: Statistical analysis of data using one way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Post hoc analysis using the Least Significant Difference (LSD). The decision criterion for statistical tests of significance was p<0.05. RESULTS
Our data shows that in step-through latencies 48 hour after training were significant
difference between rats received 20 mg kg-1 day-1 Zn Cl2
and control group (p<0.05). Furthermore in this step there was significant
difference between rats received (30, 50 mg kg-1 day-1)
Zn Cl2 with control group as respectively shown (p<0.001; p<0.01).
There were no significant differences between control group and rats which received
(70, 100 mg kg-1 day-1) (Fig. 1). But
30 days after training there was no significant difference between control group
and any of groups which received zinc chloride in different doses (Fig.
2).
| Fig. 1: |
Effect of zinc chloride on step-through latency 48 h (2 days)
after training, *p<0.05, n = 10, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001 |
| Fig. 2: |
Effect of zinc chloride on step-through latency one month
(30 days) after training, n = 10 |
| Fig. 3: |
Effect of zinc chloride on time spent in the dark chamber
48 h (2 day) after training, *p<0.05, n = 10, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001 |
On the other hand, statistical analysis of data in the time spent in the dark
chamber 48 h after training, showed significant difference between rats received
20 mg kg-1 day-1 ZnCl2 and control group (p<0.01)
and rats received 30 mg kg-1 day-1 Zn Cl2,
showed significant difference with control group (p<0.001). But in this step,
there were no significant difference between control group and rats which received
(50, 70 mg kg-1 day-1) and significant difference between
rats received 100 mg kg-1 day-1 ZnCl2 and control
group (p<0.05) (Fig. 3). But 30 days after training, there
was no significant difference between control group and any of groups which
received zinc chloride in different doses (Fig. 4).
| Fig. 4: |
Effect of zinc chloride on time spent in the dark chamber
one month (30 days) after training |
DISCUSSION
The effect of zinc chloride on learning task and memory is controversial. Furthermore,
there are different reports about the effect of this essential nutrient, on
cognitive behavior and memory, which pointed to some of them. In the present
study, young rats (aged 27-30 days) consumed zinc chloride drinking water in
five different doses (20, 30, 50, 70 and 100 mg kg-1 day-1)
for two weeks by gavage. After two weeks of oral administration, the passive
avoidance learning tested by shuttle box. The result of this research shows,
zinc consumption 30 mg kg-1 day-1 for two weeks, could
have be effective on working memory in rats than other doses (Fig.
1-3) (p<0.001). This result is as similar as the result
of many other investigators. For instance, as shown by Takeda et al.
(2000), a deficiency of this nutrient in animals resulted in malformations and
abnormal development and functioning of the central nervous system of the offspring.
In another article, this scientist reported that zinc defiance in both humans
and animals lead to impairment on passive avoidance learning, this result confirmed
present results in this experiment.
Furthermore, Kelleher et al. (2005) had shown, regulation of NMDA receptor
(which is an important receptor in learning tasks and that named learning channel)
was controlled by zinc and zinc deficiency can impair learning and memory later
in life may be by reducing NMDA receptors; however, effects of zinc deficiency
on the regulation of NMDA receptor activity are not well understood. As reported
from Eugenio and other scientist, zinc is transported from extral cellular compartments
into the neuronal and glial cells mainly via zinc transporters and transferred
to various cellular components to regulate some biological functions including
the activity of transcriptional factors involved in the in the oxidative stress
response and DNA repair. In glutamatergic neurons, zinc can also be accumulated
into synaptic vesicles and released; into order to modulate directly NMDA and
GABA receptors (Eugenio et al., 2005; Brown and Dyck, 2005; Li et
al., 2004), both of these receptors are essential for memory functions.
On the other hand, an intriguing aspect related to intracellular zinc ion availability
in the Long-Term Potentiation (LTP) is a form of synaptic plasticity, which
is implicated as a cellular mechanism subservient learning and memory (Baskys
et al., 2000). The LTP induction, at the mossy fiber-CA3 synapses, is
regulated by the release of zinc and by the subsequent entry of zinc into postsynaptic
neurons (Li et al., 2001; Eugenio et al., 2005). Zinc deficiency
may damage the learning-memory ability of the rats; the effects might be related
to the low of CCK and NOS positive neurons in hippocampal CA1 and CA3 area in
zinc deficiency rats (Li et al., 2004). Oxidative stress is associated
with the development and progression of several different neuropathologies,
including Alzheimers disease and Parkinsons disease. Zinc is maintaining
the integrity of the Blood Brain Barrier (BBB) by excluding toxic agents such
as aluminum and other foreign compounds. Alterations or dysfunction of the BBB
have been observed in many brain disorders. Zinc protects the BBB against oxidative
stress through its antioxidant properties and in so doing, helps to maintain
homeostasis within the brain and prevent the development of neurological disorders
(Karen and Julie, 2001). But some researches indicate that removing zinc from
synaptic vesicles doesnt impair spatial learning, memory, or sensorimotor
functions in the mouse. The neuromodulatory effects of zinc are not relevant
for the tasks tested, or the mice are able to compensate easily for the absence
of synaptic vesicle zinc (Toby et al., 2001). On the other hand, some
scientists had shown that enhanced zinc consumption causes memory deficits and
increased brain levels of zinc, because the influx of toxic amounts of zinc
from pre-synaptic vesicles into post-synaptic degeneration neurons seems to
be mainly responsible for the neurodegenerative process (Tonder et al.,
1990). Many scientists in their investigation have shown that zinc doesnt
affect in long-term memory. That confirms our results in this research with
100 mg kg-1 day-1 zinc chloride consumption (Edwars et
al., 2000; Flinn et al., 2005). In other research Tommie et al.
had shown that, high doses of zinc chloride lead to decline in spatial memory
in rats and this decline may be partly explained by an alteration in brain dopamine
D1 binding kinetics.
|
REFERENCES |
1: Arlene, C., W.A. Rodringuez and T.P. Wright, 1997. Time-dependent effects of fruetose on the modulation of reactivated memory. Prog. Neuro Psymemory, 21: 649-658.
2: Baskys, A., J.N. Reynolds and P.L. Carlen, 2000. NMDA depolarizations and long-term potentiation are reduced in the aged rat neocortex. Brain Res., 530: 142-146.
3: Brown, C.E. and R.H. Dyck, 2005. Modulation of synaptic zinc in barrel cortex by whisker stimulation. J. Neuro. Sci., 134: 355-359. CrossRef | Direct Link |
4: Chu, Y., M.F. Mouat and A. Grider, 2003. Water maze performance and changes in serum corticosterone levels in zinc-deprived and pair-fed rats. J. Physiol. Behave., 78: 569-578. Direct Link |
5: Edward, S., D. Halas and D. Hunt, 2000. Learning and memory disabilities in young adult rats from mildly zinc deficient dams. J. Physiol. Behav., 37: 451-458.
6: Eugenio, M., B.F. Carlo, M. Fiorella and M. Malavolta, 2005. Brain, aging and neurodegeneration: Role of zinc ion availability. J. Prog. Neurobiol., 75: 367-390. PubMed | Direct Link |
7: Filinn, J.M., D. Hunter and D.H. Kinkous et al., 2005. Enhanced zinc consumption causes memory deficits and increased brain levels of zinc. J. Phis. Behav., 83: 793-803. CrossRef | Direct Link |
8: Karen, A. and A. Julie, 2001. Supplementation with L-histidine during dietary zinc repletion improves short-term memory in zinc-restricted young adult male rats. J. Nutr., 130: 1633-1640.
9: Chowanadisai, W., S.L. Kelleher and M. Lonnerdal, 2005. Maternal zinc deficiency reduces NMDA recertor expression in neonatal rat brain, which persists into early, adulthood. J. Nerochem., 94: 510-519. CrossRef | Direct Link |
10: Li, Y., C.J. Hough, C.J. Frederickson, J.F. Christopher, and M.S. John, 2001. Induction of mossy fiber? CA3 long-term potentiation requires translocation of synaptically released zinc. J. Neuro. Sci., 21: 8015-8025. Direct Link |
11: Li, J., B. Yan, X. Zhao and C. Wang, 2004. Changes of cholecystokinin and nitric oxide synthase positive neurons of hippocampus in zinc-deficiency rat. J. Neuro. Sci., 33: 559-561. PubMed | Direct Link |
12: Maureen, M., S. Sunil, K. Sonu, J. Kumar and V. Menon, 2004. Cognitive and motor development among small-for-gestational-age infants: Impact of zinc supplementation, birth weight and caregiving practices. J. Pediatr., 113: 1297-1304. Direct Link |
13: Ono, S. and M.G. Cherian, 1999. Regional distribution of metallothionein, zinc and copper in the brain of different strains of rats. Biol. Trace Elem., 69: 151-159. CrossRef | Direct Link |
14: Pang, Z. and D.S. Wang, 1999. Zinc depresses GABA receptor mediated responses in acutely dissociated sacral commissural neuron. Shey. Bao So., 50: 649-655.
15: Patenaude, C., S. Nurse and J.C. Lacaille, 2002. Sensitivity of synaptic GABA receptor to allosteric modulators in hippocampal oriens-alveus interneurons. J. Synapse., 41: 29-39.
16: Pfeiffer, C. and E. Braveman, 2000. Zinc, brain and behavior. J. Biol. Psych., 17: 513-532.
17: Penland, J.G., H.H. Sandstead, N.W. Alcock, H.H. Dayal and X.C. Chen et al., 1997. A preliminary report: Effects of zinc and micronutrient repletion on growth and neuropsychological function of urban Chinese children. J. Am. Coll. Nutr., 16: 268-272. Direct Link |
18: Takeda, A., 2000. Movement of zinc and its functional significant in the brain. J. Brain. Res. Rev., 34: 137-148. CrossRef | Direct Link |
19: Takeda, A., S. Takefuta and S. Takefuta et al., 2000. Relationship between brain zinc and transient learning impairment of adult rats fed zinc-deficiencent dite. J. Brain. Res., 859: 352-357. CrossRef | Direct Link |
20: Takeda, A., A. Minami and Y. Seki, 2004. Differential effects of zinc on glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmitter systems in the hippocampus under zinc deficiency. J. Neurosci. Res., 75: 225-229.
21: Takeda, A., H. Tamano, M. Tochigi and N. Oku, 2005. Zinc homeostasis in the hippocampus of zinc-deficient young adult rats. Neurochem. Int., 46: 221-225. CrossRef | PubMed |
22: Cole, T.B., A. Martyanova and R.D. Palmiter, 2001. Removing zinc from synaptic vesicles dose not impair spation learning, memory, or sensorimotor function in the mouse. J. Brain. Res., 891: 253-265. Direct Link |
23: Turner, T.Y. and M. Soliman, 2000. Effects of zinc on spatial reference memory and brain dopamine (D1) receptor binding kinetics in rats. J. Nero. Psychol. Pharma., 24: 1203-1217. CrossRef | Direct Link |
24: Tonder, N., F.F. Johansen, C. Erickson, J. Zimmer and N.H. Diemer, 1990. Role of zinc in the selective degeneration of dentate hilar neurons after cerebral ischemia in the adult rat. J. Neuro Sci., 109: 247-252.
25: Tuula, E., 1995. The adverse effects of zinc deficiency. J. Orthomol. Med. Rev., 10: 149-164.
|
|
|
 |